ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics

Autores
Martino, Daniela Lorena
Año de publicación
2017
Idioma
inglés
Tipo de recurso
documento de conferencia
Estado
versión publicada
Descripción
The different perspectives that aim at explaining how pronunciation in a foreign language is acquired are key to developing activities that foster effective learning. In an attempt to explain the struggles faced by students when it comes to pronunciation in a foreign language, special attention has been given to the first language and the way it might interfere with the acquisition of L2/FL sounds. Polivanov (1931, 1964) and Trubetzkoy (1939, 1969) focused on the effect of speech perception on phonology arguing that sounds in a second language are perceived through the phonological system of the first language and failing to articulate a certain sound was the result of perceiving it erroneously. In this way the authors pointed out that inexact L2 production is the result of erroneous perception, suggesting that L1 operates as a phonological filter through which the L2 sounds are processed. In the same line, Dupoux (2002, p. 172) described the “phonological deafness” that learners suffer from as they strive to discriminate sounds in a foreign language using a “processing apparatus specifically tuned to their maternal language” bringing about a lot of difficulty “in dealing with sound structures that are alien to the language they heard as infants”. Flege (2003) proposes the speech learning model (SLM) which posits that the L1 and L2 phonetic subsystems influence each other as a result of coexisting in a common phonological space leading to L1 and L2 sounds either merging into category assimilation or shifting away from each other (category dissimilation). The need to lay particular emphasis on learners’ perception becomes evident from the previously mentioned theories. It is interesting to note that L2 learners might be able to discriminate the acoustic difference between phones used to realize two categories on an auditory basis but may fail to do so phonetically. Therefore, perceptual awareness of categorical distinctions would seem to take place before the ability to implement such distinctions, with time and practice as key to establishing “the motor control patterns needed for producing new phones in an L2” (Flege, 1987, p. 290). This explains the practice stage that should follow the work on perception. In connection to the motor control referred above, the concept of proprioception –internal kinesthetic awareness of the position and movement of our muscles and parts of the body– takes on paramount importance. Listen and repeat exercises on their own fail to help students develop “proprioceptive or kinesthetic intelligence that can gradually liberate the learner from the oral and aural grip of their mother tongue pronunciation habits” (Underhill, 2012). Pronunciation, being a physical and muscular activity –a motor skill–, should not be taught on a purely cognitive basis. The research done on the acquisition of L2/EF pronunciation serves as framework for the design of a set of stages and activities that can help learners advance from controlled to automatic processing (Celce-Murcia, Brinton & Goodwin, 2006). The advance of technology provides learners with an array of tools that can not only aid but also accompany learners in such process.
Trabajo publicado en Caldiz, A. y Rafaelli, V. (coords.) (2020). Exploraciones fonolingüísticas. V Jornadas Internacionales de Fonética y Fonología y I Jornadas Nacionales de Fonética y Discurso.
Facultad de Humanidades y Ciencias de la Educación
Materia
Humanidades
Letras
pronunciation
phonetics
Nivel de accesibilidad
acceso abierto
Condiciones de uso
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
Repositorio
SEDICI (UNLP)
Institución
Universidad Nacional de La Plata
OAI Identificador
oai:sedici.unlp.edu.ar:10915/130242

id SEDICI_51b7b5c90e6ae35d7dfad45a2227b981
oai_identifier_str oai:sedici.unlp.edu.ar:10915/130242
network_acronym_str SEDICI
repository_id_str 1329
network_name_str SEDICI (UNLP)
spelling ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phoneticsMartino, Daniela LorenaHumanidadesLetraspronunciationphoneticsThe different perspectives that aim at explaining how pronunciation in a foreign language is acquired are key to developing activities that foster effective learning. In an attempt to explain the struggles faced by students when it comes to pronunciation in a foreign language, special attention has been given to the first language and the way it might interfere with the acquisition of L2/FL sounds. Polivanov (1931, 1964) and Trubetzkoy (1939, 1969) focused on the effect of speech perception on phonology arguing that sounds in a second language are perceived through the phonological system of the first language and failing to articulate a certain sound was the result of perceiving it erroneously. In this way the authors pointed out that inexact L2 production is the result of erroneous perception, suggesting that L1 operates as a phonological filter through which the L2 sounds are processed. In the same line, Dupoux (2002, p. 172) described the “phonological deafness” that learners suffer from as they strive to discriminate sounds in a foreign language using a “processing apparatus specifically tuned to their maternal language” bringing about a lot of difficulty “in dealing with sound structures that are alien to the language they heard as infants”. Flege (2003) proposes the speech learning model (SLM) which posits that the L1 and L2 phonetic subsystems influence each other as a result of coexisting in a common phonological space leading to L1 and L2 sounds either merging into category assimilation or shifting away from each other (category dissimilation). The need to lay particular emphasis on learners’ perception becomes evident from the previously mentioned theories. It is interesting to note that L2 learners might be able to discriminate the acoustic difference between phones used to realize two categories on an auditory basis but may fail to do so phonetically. Therefore, perceptual awareness of categorical distinctions would seem to take place before the ability to implement such distinctions, with time and practice as key to establishing “the motor control patterns needed for producing new phones in an L2” (Flege, 1987, p. 290). This explains the practice stage that should follow the work on perception. In connection to the motor control referred above, the concept of proprioception –internal kinesthetic awareness of the position and movement of our muscles and parts of the body– takes on paramount importance. Listen and repeat exercises on their own fail to help students develop “proprioceptive or kinesthetic intelligence that can gradually liberate the learner from the oral and aural grip of their mother tongue pronunciation habits” (Underhill, 2012). Pronunciation, being a physical and muscular activity –a motor skill–, should not be taught on a purely cognitive basis. The research done on the acquisition of L2/EF pronunciation serves as framework for the design of a set of stages and activities that can help learners advance from controlled to automatic processing (Celce-Murcia, Brinton & Goodwin, 2006). The advance of technology provides learners with an array of tools that can not only aid but also accompany learners in such process.Trabajo publicado en Caldiz, A. y Rafaelli, V. (coords.) (2020). <i>Exploraciones fonolingüísticas. V Jornadas Internacionales de Fonética y Fonología y I Jornadas Nacionales de Fonética y Discurso</i>.Facultad de Humanidades y Ciencias de la Educación2017-09info:eu-repo/semantics/conferenceObjectinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersionObjeto de conferenciahttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_5794info:ar-repo/semantics/documentoDeConferenciaapplication/pdf523-534http://sedici.unlp.edu.ar/handle/10915/130242enginfo:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/isbn/978-950-34-1943-4info:eu-repo/semantics/reference/hdl/10915/113217info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesshttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0)reponame:SEDICI (UNLP)instname:Universidad Nacional de La Platainstacron:UNLP2025-09-03T11:04:52Zoai:sedici.unlp.edu.ar:10915/130242Institucionalhttp://sedici.unlp.edu.ar/Universidad públicaNo correspondehttp://sedici.unlp.edu.ar/oai/snrdalira@sedici.unlp.edu.arArgentinaNo correspondeNo correspondeNo correspondeopendoar:13292025-09-03 11:04:53.076SEDICI (UNLP) - Universidad Nacional de La Platafalse
dc.title.none.fl_str_mv ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
title ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
spellingShingle ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
Martino, Daniela Lorena
Humanidades
Letras
pronunciation
phonetics
title_short ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
title_full ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
title_fullStr ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
title_full_unstemmed ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
title_sort ICT aided steps in the learning of segmental phonetics
dc.creator.none.fl_str_mv Martino, Daniela Lorena
author Martino, Daniela Lorena
author_facet Martino, Daniela Lorena
author_role author
dc.subject.none.fl_str_mv Humanidades
Letras
pronunciation
phonetics
topic Humanidades
Letras
pronunciation
phonetics
dc.description.none.fl_txt_mv The different perspectives that aim at explaining how pronunciation in a foreign language is acquired are key to developing activities that foster effective learning. In an attempt to explain the struggles faced by students when it comes to pronunciation in a foreign language, special attention has been given to the first language and the way it might interfere with the acquisition of L2/FL sounds. Polivanov (1931, 1964) and Trubetzkoy (1939, 1969) focused on the effect of speech perception on phonology arguing that sounds in a second language are perceived through the phonological system of the first language and failing to articulate a certain sound was the result of perceiving it erroneously. In this way the authors pointed out that inexact L2 production is the result of erroneous perception, suggesting that L1 operates as a phonological filter through which the L2 sounds are processed. In the same line, Dupoux (2002, p. 172) described the “phonological deafness” that learners suffer from as they strive to discriminate sounds in a foreign language using a “processing apparatus specifically tuned to their maternal language” bringing about a lot of difficulty “in dealing with sound structures that are alien to the language they heard as infants”. Flege (2003) proposes the speech learning model (SLM) which posits that the L1 and L2 phonetic subsystems influence each other as a result of coexisting in a common phonological space leading to L1 and L2 sounds either merging into category assimilation or shifting away from each other (category dissimilation). The need to lay particular emphasis on learners’ perception becomes evident from the previously mentioned theories. It is interesting to note that L2 learners might be able to discriminate the acoustic difference between phones used to realize two categories on an auditory basis but may fail to do so phonetically. Therefore, perceptual awareness of categorical distinctions would seem to take place before the ability to implement such distinctions, with time and practice as key to establishing “the motor control patterns needed for producing new phones in an L2” (Flege, 1987, p. 290). This explains the practice stage that should follow the work on perception. In connection to the motor control referred above, the concept of proprioception –internal kinesthetic awareness of the position and movement of our muscles and parts of the body– takes on paramount importance. Listen and repeat exercises on their own fail to help students develop “proprioceptive or kinesthetic intelligence that can gradually liberate the learner from the oral and aural grip of their mother tongue pronunciation habits” (Underhill, 2012). Pronunciation, being a physical and muscular activity –a motor skill–, should not be taught on a purely cognitive basis. The research done on the acquisition of L2/EF pronunciation serves as framework for the design of a set of stages and activities that can help learners advance from controlled to automatic processing (Celce-Murcia, Brinton & Goodwin, 2006). The advance of technology provides learners with an array of tools that can not only aid but also accompany learners in such process.
Trabajo publicado en Caldiz, A. y Rafaelli, V. (coords.) (2020). <i>Exploraciones fonolingüísticas. V Jornadas Internacionales de Fonética y Fonología y I Jornadas Nacionales de Fonética y Discurso</i>.
Facultad de Humanidades y Ciencias de la Educación
description The different perspectives that aim at explaining how pronunciation in a foreign language is acquired are key to developing activities that foster effective learning. In an attempt to explain the struggles faced by students when it comes to pronunciation in a foreign language, special attention has been given to the first language and the way it might interfere with the acquisition of L2/FL sounds. Polivanov (1931, 1964) and Trubetzkoy (1939, 1969) focused on the effect of speech perception on phonology arguing that sounds in a second language are perceived through the phonological system of the first language and failing to articulate a certain sound was the result of perceiving it erroneously. In this way the authors pointed out that inexact L2 production is the result of erroneous perception, suggesting that L1 operates as a phonological filter through which the L2 sounds are processed. In the same line, Dupoux (2002, p. 172) described the “phonological deafness” that learners suffer from as they strive to discriminate sounds in a foreign language using a “processing apparatus specifically tuned to their maternal language” bringing about a lot of difficulty “in dealing with sound structures that are alien to the language they heard as infants”. Flege (2003) proposes the speech learning model (SLM) which posits that the L1 and L2 phonetic subsystems influence each other as a result of coexisting in a common phonological space leading to L1 and L2 sounds either merging into category assimilation or shifting away from each other (category dissimilation). The need to lay particular emphasis on learners’ perception becomes evident from the previously mentioned theories. It is interesting to note that L2 learners might be able to discriminate the acoustic difference between phones used to realize two categories on an auditory basis but may fail to do so phonetically. Therefore, perceptual awareness of categorical distinctions would seem to take place before the ability to implement such distinctions, with time and practice as key to establishing “the motor control patterns needed for producing new phones in an L2” (Flege, 1987, p. 290). This explains the practice stage that should follow the work on perception. In connection to the motor control referred above, the concept of proprioception –internal kinesthetic awareness of the position and movement of our muscles and parts of the body– takes on paramount importance. Listen and repeat exercises on their own fail to help students develop “proprioceptive or kinesthetic intelligence that can gradually liberate the learner from the oral and aural grip of their mother tongue pronunciation habits” (Underhill, 2012). Pronunciation, being a physical and muscular activity –a motor skill–, should not be taught on a purely cognitive basis. The research done on the acquisition of L2/EF pronunciation serves as framework for the design of a set of stages and activities that can help learners advance from controlled to automatic processing (Celce-Murcia, Brinton & Goodwin, 2006). The advance of technology provides learners with an array of tools that can not only aid but also accompany learners in such process.
publishDate 2017
dc.date.none.fl_str_mv 2017-09
dc.type.none.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/conferenceObject
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
Objeto de conferencia
http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_5794
info:ar-repo/semantics/documentoDeConferencia
format conferenceObject
status_str publishedVersion
dc.identifier.none.fl_str_mv http://sedici.unlp.edu.ar/handle/10915/130242
url http://sedici.unlp.edu.ar/handle/10915/130242
dc.language.none.fl_str_mv eng
language eng
dc.relation.none.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/isbn/978-950-34-1943-4
info:eu-repo/semantics/reference/hdl/10915/113217
dc.rights.none.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0)
eu_rights_str_mv openAccess
rights_invalid_str_mv http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0)
dc.format.none.fl_str_mv application/pdf
523-534
dc.source.none.fl_str_mv reponame:SEDICI (UNLP)
instname:Universidad Nacional de La Plata
instacron:UNLP
reponame_str SEDICI (UNLP)
collection SEDICI (UNLP)
instname_str Universidad Nacional de La Plata
instacron_str UNLP
institution UNLP
repository.name.fl_str_mv SEDICI (UNLP) - Universidad Nacional de La Plata
repository.mail.fl_str_mv alira@sedici.unlp.edu.ar
_version_ 1842260548987650048
score 13.13397